Submitted March 12, 1996 Resubmitted April 1, 1996 Resubmitted April 26, 1996 Accepted April 29, 1996 Publication Date: May 14, 1996 "FEMININE SPEECH" IN HOMOGENEOUS GENDER GROUPS* Robert K. Shelly Ohio University ABSTRACT A characteristic "feminine speech" style has been identified as an ideal type, either using tag questions or rising inflections at the end of declarative sentences or actually stating task contributions as questions. This style has been characterized as part of the feminine role repertoire socialized in American society. Data from seventy three-person open interaction task groups are used to test the idea that this behavior pattern is a role style, with women enacting the behavior more than men. Results indicate that men and women initiate this behavior at a similar rate in the homogeneous gender groups studied, suggesting that this behavior reflects status patterns of the larger society enacted in heterogeneous task settings, but not in homogeneous gender settings. [page 50] - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - [page 51] INTRODUCTION Lakoff (1975) and Tannen (1994) describe a feminine interaction style with verbal characteristics distinct from a masculine interaction style. These characteristics include a greater use of verbal tags such as "...don't you think?" or "That is a good idea, isn't it?" Women are also thought to be more likely to employ verbalizations that imply a question, with rising inflection placed on the end of a declarative statement. But how and under what conditions is this set of behavior observed? Is it truly a feminine pattern of behavior, or is it observable in particular situations in both genders? This paper examines interaction in homogeneous task groups to answer the question of whether the behavior is a "feminine style" or is observable for both females and males under different circumstances. Researchers report a variety of effects of gender in interaction in task groups. These effects, involving both verbal and nonverbal behavior, have been reported in heterogeneous and homogeneous groups. In general, men are reported to be more task-oriented than women (Strodtbeck and Mann, 1956; Nemeth, Endicott, and Wachtler, 1976; Aries, 1976; Borgatta and Stimson, 1963; Craig and Sheriff, 1986; Kelley, Wildman, and Urey, 1982; Lockheed and Hall, 1976; Mabry, 1985; Piliavin and Martin, 1978; Wood and Karten, 1986), and more likely to display high status interaction cues such as chin thrusts and looking while speaking than women (Dovidio, Brown, Heltman, Ellyson, and Keating, 1988; Dovidio, Ellyson, Keating, Heltman, and Brown, 1988; Carli, 1990). Women are more likely to engage in socioemotional behavior, be more concerned with social process (Piliavin and Martin, 1978; Lakoff, 1975; Tannen, 1995), smile more, and display low status cues such as looking while listening and withdrawn posture (Dovidio, et al., 1988; Dovidio, et al., 1988). EXPLANATIONS Two explanations have been advanced for the research results reviewed above. One emphasizes the feminine role, with an associated interaction style (Lakoff, 1975; Eagly and Karan, 1991; Maccoby, 1990; Tannen, 1995). This explanation asserts men and women are socialized to play particular gender roles in society, that this socialization creates trans-situational behavior repertoires, and that these repertoires are activated in most interaction situations, regardless of the situation or gender composition of the groups in the situation. These behavior repertoires include verbal behaviors, such as the use of tag questions and the demonstration of a willingness to agree with assertions of others, and nonverbal behaviors, such as smiling, gaze, and posture. [page 51] - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - [page 52] The other explanation asserts that observed differences in both interpersonal behavior and interaction cues such as verbal style, posture, and gaze are due to the instantiation of status differences from the larger society into the task group. Thus, the relative advantages of males and females in society are reflected in task groups, with females enacting a low status role and males enacting a high status role in situations in heterogeneous groups. In homogeneous groups, males and females should exhibit no differences in behaviors or interaction style. This approach has been developed by Berger and others in expectation states theory (Wagner and Berger, 1995; Ridgeway, 1988; 1991; Ridgeway and Diekema, 1992; Shelly and Munroe, 1994). According to this explanation, behavioral differences are observed when status differences are activated in task groups. One situation in which such differences are activated occurs when males and females are in heterogeneous task groups. Because of their higher social standing, males exhibit more high-status verbal and nonverbal behaviors than females in heterogeneous groups. Males talk more, exert influence, look more while speaking, and sit forward in their chairs. Similarly, females talk less, accept influence, look while listening, and sit back in their chairs. In homogeneous groups, there should be no difference in verbal or nonverbal behavior due to the gender of the participants in the groups. Males and females should talk the same amount, be equally likely to exercise influence or be influenced, look while speaking, or look while listening. Other status organizing structures may produce these effects, but gender should not lead to behavioral differences in homogeneous groups. According to expectation states theory, the distinct interaction style associated with females in heterogeneous groups is a status effect, due to a lower standing of women in society and not a socialized pattern of role behavior. But, if gender is not activated in task groups, then males and females should employ verbal acts at the same rate in homogeneous task groups. Hypothesis: In task groups, males and females should employ verbal tags, such as questions at the end of declaratives and rising tone of voice at the end of declaratives, at different rates. That is, males should employ this behavior less than females. [page 52] - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - [page 53] METHOD The data used to test the hypothesis consist of observations of interaction in seventy gender-homogeneous, three-person task groups (thirty-four male groups, thirty-six female groups). The groups were composed of undergraduate students at a state university who met to solve the NASA "Lost on the Moon Exercise" (Johnson and Johnson, 1994). The groups were distributed equally across seven conditions designed to test how different imposed social structures affect interaction. The conditions included a control condition in which no manipulation was carried out, a condition in which liking for others was manipulated, a condition in which an authority position was created, a condition in which skills of participants were varied, a condition in which the authority position was occupied by a highly skilled person, a condition in which the authority position was occupied by a well liked person, and a condition in which the skilled person was well liked. A more detailed discussion of the techniques employed in the experiment appears in Shelly and Munroe (1994). The independent variables are the sex of the group members, the condition of the experiment as an organizing structure, and the position of the actor (advantage with respect to others in the group). Measures of interaction were coded from video tapes of each group. Discussion lasted from ten to twenty minutes per group. All discussions concluded when consensus had been reached about the best solution to the problem. The coding identified statements with the following properties as verbal tags (TAG):(1) a declarative content, but ending with a tag question (e.g., "I think we should put the rope next, don't you?"); (2) statements that are declarative in content, but ending with a rising inflection (e.g., "I think the gun goes next (rise in tone of voice)."); (3) a response to a question that contains a suggestion for a task solution, but phrased as a question (e.g., (Q) "What should we put next?" (A) "What about the rope?"); and (4) a directly stated question that offers a solution to a task problem (e.g., "Should we put the rope next?"). Two individuals coded independently of one another. The reliability analysis compared the constituent parts of the coding to one another and the total tagged activity, arriving at a Cronbach's alpha of .775. This is a moderate reliability compared to other studies of face-to- face interaction that report values above .90. RESULTS The individual measures of tagged interaction were summed to arrive at a total number of tags per actor (TAG). This ranged from zero to fifteen acts, with a mean of 3.64 and a median of 3.0. [page 53] - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - [page 54] Two other measures may be created from this measure and its place in the total interaction in the group. One is the percent of each actor's own initiations that are tagged (PCTTAG), which measures the extent to which a person is consistently deferential to others in a task group. The range for this measure is zero to twenty seven percent, with a mean of 4.66 and a median of 3.74. The second measure is the percent of total activity in a group accounted for by tagged activity by an individual, which indicates how much an individual might stand out by using tagged speech in interaction (TAGPCTTOT). This variable has a range from zero to thirty one percent, with a mean of 2.29 and median of 1.44. The simplest test of the hypothesis is to compare means for males and females for tagged speech (TAG). Comparing means for the percent of own speech that is tagged (PCTTAG) provides a test normed on the actor. Comparing means for the percent t of total group speech tagged (TAGPCTTOT) allows a test of the hypothesis normed on the group. The results of these tests are shown in Table 1. Table 1. Means of tagged speech measures by sex. Sex TAG PCTTAG TAGPCTTOT Male 3.43 4.92 2.73 (N=102) Female 3.84 4.40 1.87 (N=108) T-test t=-1.02 t=.95 t=1.85 p=.308 p=.342 p=.066 It is clear that in these homogeneous groups, females do not generate significantly more verbal tags than males. This is true for the frequency with which this behavior is generated (TAG). Females initiate slightly more behavior of this sort, but a goodness of fit interpretation of the t-test would suggest that this behavior is initiated at the same rate for each gender (p=.308). For both percentage measures, males initiate more activity, though the results are not significantly different for either measure. A goodness of fit interpretations for the percent of a person's activity generated (PCTTAG) would suggest no difference between males and females on this behavioral measure (p=.342). Males initiate a higher percentage of total interaction than females as tagged activity (TAGPCTTOT), though this difference is not significant at the usual level. [page 54] - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - [page 55] If a directional hypothesis is formulated for tagged activity as a percentage of total interaction, with males expected to generate more of this activity than females, the result (males=2.73% versus females=1.87%) is significant at the .05 level. This is a direct reversal of the gender hypothesis I set out to test. The possibility of interactions between the sex of the group and the imposed structure is examined to determine if females and males behave differently in some situations but not others. An analysis of variance tests this hypothesis. The ANOVA identifies two statistically significant main effects and one significant interaction between gender and condition. One main effect is for position: being in the relative advantaged position versus the disadvantaged position makes it more likely the actor will engage in tagged speech (X(a)=4.17 versus X(d)=3.38, F=5.494; 1 d.f., p=.02). There is also a statistically significant effect for condition of the experiment, with tagged behavior more likely in Skill at task and Authority and Skill conditions (F=7.062, 6 d.f., p=.00). Table 2 presents the results for TAG for such an analysis. Analysis of the PCTTAG and TAGPCTTOT produce similar results. Table 2. Means of tagged speech (TAG) by sex and imposed structure. Condition Total Tags Male Female Control 3.93 3.40 4.47 (N=15) (N=15) Formal 2.37 2.33 2.42 Authority (N=18) (N=12) Liking 3.13 2.58 3.50 (N=12) (N=18) Skill 5.30*** 2.53 8.07*,** at task (N=15) (N=15) Authority 2.50 2.00 3.00 and Liking (N=15) (N=15) Skill 3.37 3.73 3.00 and Liking (N=15) (N=15) Authority 4.90*** 8.50*,** 2.50 and Skill (N=12) (N=18) * Significantly different from the opposite sex actor in this condition. This comparison is by Tukey test for significant differences. ** Significantly different from the other conditions in this sex. This comparison is by Tukey test for significant differences. *** The mean for the Skill condition is significantly different than the means for the Authority, Authority and Liking, and Liking Condition. The mean for the Authority and Skill condition is significantly different from the means for the Authority and Authority and Liking Conditions. This comparison is by Tukey test for differences. [page 55] - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - [page 56] The introduction of manipulations for structures in these groups does not alter the result that males and females do not differ in the rate at which they initiate tagged speech. But there are two conditions for which this is not true. Females in the Skill condition initiate more tagged speech than either males in this condition or females in other structures. Males in the Authority and Skill condition initiate more tagged speech in this condition than males in other conditions as well. Controlling for the rate of initiation by examining the percentage of tagged speech does not alter these conclusions for PCTTAG. There is an interaction effect observable in the Authority condition for TAGPCTTOT. Females assigned to the Authority condition initiate more tagged speech than do males as a percentage of their total interaction (.85 percent of total activity for males versus 1.59 percent of total activity for females). Overall, males and females initiate tagged speech at the same rate in these homogeneous gender task groups. Effects observed in imposed social structures show advantaged actors employ the speech style more than disadvantaged actors. Interactions with gender and imposed structures show complementary results with females engaging in this behavior more in one condition of the experiment and males in another. Differences between males and females are not significant in the other five conditions of the study. Females initiate more tagged behavior as a percentage of total interaction, but in only one condition of the experiment. The task of explaining these results remains. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS The question I set out to answer was whether tagged speech can be thought of as a distinct interaction style, characteristic of one gender. The alternative interpretation is that such speech distinguishes one gender from another only when gender is activated as a status characteristic. This alternative suggests males and females in task groups will exhibit such interaction patterns differentially only in heterogeneous groups, and not in homogeneous groups. This is the overall finding for the seventy groups studied for this report: males and females behave the same in this study. Males and females initiate tagged speech, the "feminine style," at the same rate in homogeneous groups, no matter how the activity is measured (simple frequency, normed on the actor, or normed on the group). Some interaction effects are observed in various imposed social structures, but these effects are observed in male groups in one structure and in female groups in another structure. There are no differences in rate of initiation of tagged speech in the other five types of groups. Thus the conclusion is that tagged speech is not a "feminine speech" style socialized in gender roles. [page 56] - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - [page 57] Two explanations for these results are possible. One is that actors seek to "sugarcoat" task activity if legitimation of task action is suspect. This would account for the results cited by Tannen (1994) and others, and is consistent with the results described above. The activity would thus have differential meaning as suggested by Ridgeway and Diekema (1992): high status actors employ the behavior under some conditions, and low status actors employ it under other circumstances. Another possibility is that tagged behavior is a status cue (Berger, Webster, Ridgeway, and Rosenholtz, 1986). Task cues, verbal and nonverbal acts, provide information about an actor's capacity to perform a task. Tagged behavior communicates such information (Newcombe and Arnkoff, 1979). A well designed experiment in which male and female actors reproduce tagged and untagged speech patterns in attempting to influence subjects would provide a strong test of tagged speech as a status cue. Such a study would advance our understanding of how men and women interact with members of the same sex and members of the opposite sex and the meanings they attach to verbal and nonverbal interaction behavior. ENDNOTE *Research reported here was supported in part by grants from Ohio University Research Challenge Funds and from DAAL03-86-D-0001 from the Naval Training Systems Center. Views, opinions, and findings contained in this report are those of the author and should not be construed as an official Department of the Army position, policy or decision. REFERENCES Aries, Elizabeth J. 1976. Interaction patterns and themes of male and female,and mixed sex groups. Small Group Behavior 7: 7-18. Berger, Joseph, Murray Webster, Jr., Cecilia L. Ridgeway, and Susan Rosenholtz. 1986. Status Cues, Expectations, and Behavior. In Edward J. Lawler (Editor) Advances in Group Processes. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press, Inc. Borgatta, Edgar F. and John Stimson. 1963. Sex differences in interaction characteristics. Journal of Social Psychology 60: 89-100. Craig, Jane M. and Carolyn W. Sherif. 1986. The effectiveness of men and women in problem-solving groups as a function of group gender composition. Sex Roles 14: 453-66. Dovidio, John F., Clifford E. Brown, Karen Heltman, Steve L. Ellyson, and Caroline F. Keating. 1988. Power displays between women and men in discussions of gender-linked tasks: A multichannel study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 55: 580-587. [page 57] - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - [page 58] Dovidio, John F., Steve L. Ellyson, Caroline F. Keating, Karen Heltman, and Clifford E. Brown. 1988. The relationship of social power to visual displays of dominance between men and women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54: 233-242. Eagly, Alice H. and Steven J. Karan. 1991. Gender and the emergence of leaders. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 60: 685-710. Johnson, David W. and Frank P. Johnson. 1994. Joining Together: Group Theory and Group Skills. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon. Kelley, Jeffrey A., Hal E. Wildman, and Jon R. Urey. 1982. Gender and sex role differences in group decision-making social interactions: a behavioral analysis. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 12: 112-127. Lakoff, Robin. 1975. Language and Women's Place. New York: Harper and Row. Lockheed, Marlaine E. and Katherine P. Hall. 1976. Conceptualizing sex as a status characteristic: Applications to leadership training strategies. Journal of Social Issues 32: 111- 124. Mabry, Edward. 1985. The effects of gender composition and task structure on small group interaction. Small Group Behavior 16: 75-96. Maccoby, Eleanor E. 1990. Gender and relationships: A developmental account. American Psychologist 45: 513-520. Nemeth, Charlan, Jeffrey Endicott, and Joel Wachtler. 1976. From the '50's to the '70's: Women in jury deliberations. Sociometry 39: 293- 304. Newcombe, Nora and Diane Arnkoff. 1979. Effects of speech style and sex of speaker on person perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 37: 1293-1303. Piliavin, Jane A. and Rachel R. Martin. 1978. The effects of sex composition of groups on style of social interaction. Sex Roles 4: 281- 96. Ridgeway, Cecilia L. 1988. Gender differences in task groups: A status and legitimacy account. In Murray A. Webster, Jr. and Martha Foschi (Editors) Status Generalization: New Theory and Research. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. -----. 1991. The social construction of status value: Gender and other nominal characteristics. Social Forces 70: 367-386. Ridgeway, Cecilia L. and David Diekema. 1992. Are gender differences status differences? In Cecilia L. Ridgeway (Editor) Gender, Interaction, and Inequality. New York: Springer-Verlag. [page 58] - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - [page 59] Shelly, Robert K. and Paul T. Munroe. 1994. Do women engage in less task behavior than men? Paper presented at the Annual Group Processes Conference. Los Angeles, California. Strodtbeck, Fred L. and Richard D. Mann. 1956. Sex role differentiation in jury deliberations. Sociometry 19: 3-11. Tannen, Deborah. 1994. Talking 9 to 5. New York: William Morrow and Company. Wagner, David G. and Joseph Berger. 1995. Gender and interpersonal behavior: Status expectation accounts. Working paper 100-10. Department of Sociology. Stanford University: Stanford, CA. Wood, Wendy and Steven J. Karten. 1986. Sex differences in interaction style as a product of perceived sex differences in competence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 50:341-347. AUTHOR BIOGRAPHY Robert K. Shelly is Professor of Sociology at Ohio University. He is continuing studies of interaction and the formation of expectations, and is currently investigating how social structures without socially defined status-value organize interaction. Address correspondence to: rshelly1@ohiou.edu